CONTOH SKRIPSI BAHASA INGGRIS IMPROVING ADOLESCENT COMPREHENSION: DEVELOPING COMPREHENSION STRATEGIES IN THE CONTENT AREAS



Improving Adolescent Comprehension: Developing Learning Strategies in Content Areas
            The field of adolescent literacy is engaged in a continual struggle with what it means to promote comprehension.  Starting out as content area reading, the field was preoccupied with developing teaching activities for learning from texts.  For nearly 20 years, from the early 1960’s until the early 1990’s, proponents of content area reading, and then content area literacy, recognizing the integrated roles of reading, writing, speaking and listening (McKenna & Robinson, 1990), churned out one teaching activity after another for fostering comprehension.  The names of these activities are ubiquitous – semantic maps and graphic organizers, anticipation guides, three-level guides, journaling, I-searches and the list goes on and on.  A compendium of these activities is in its sixth edition (Tierney & Readance, 2004).
            In the 1990’s, the field turned its attention to adolescents.  In an article documenting the shift, Lisa Patel Stevens argues for a reconceptualization of the field to include out of school literacies (Stevens, 2002).  Critical of school-based approaches to comprehension, which, according to Stevens, focus on factual comprehension of texts, she promotes adolescent multiple literacies.  This reframing poses a fundamental shift in views of comprehension to include the interaction of the learner, texts, contexts and culture.  In short, comprehension is no longer the oversimplified application of a teaching activity or task to a text, it is an ecological event characterized by the complexities of an “enactment of self” and the “interplay of multiple texts.” (Moje et al., 2000).
            Despite these huge ideological and empirical swings – at one time for teaching activity and task and then toward a celebration of the adolescent – an important point is repeatedly ignored: comprehension, especially in the content areas, is about learning and, often, doing (Conley, 2007).  Thorndike long ago recognized a very active and strategic role for readers and comprehension, including sorting and sifting, regarding some ideas as tentative and others as important, and organizing comprehension for some greater purpose, such as problem solving or communicating (Thorndike, 1917).  Pressley and his colleagues have reinforced and elaborated this view with comprehension strategies as the engine that drives comprehension (Block et al., 2002; Pressley, 2000, 2006; Pressley & Hilden, 2006).  Comprehension strategies are goal oriented processes that readers and writers use to construct meaning.  What we know about comprehension strategies and comprehension comes mostly from studies of skilled reading (Pressley, 2006; Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995; Wyatt et al., 1993) and from studies of children who experience difficulties with reading (Cain & Oakhill, 2004). The message from this research is unequivocal: skilled readers know how to select and apply comprehension strategies where and when they need them to comprehend; struggling readers experience difficulties with comprehension because they know little about comprehension strategies or how to use them.
            Research in content arealiteracy/adolescent literacy has rarely, if ever, addressed comprehension strategies, despite our growing understanding of their importance.  Some critics of content area literacy have suggested that the research is overly restricted in its focus solely on teaching activities, tasks and text meanings, leaving the role of the reader out entirely (Moje et al., 2000).  Adolescent literacy celebrates the uniqueness of adolescence combined with the potential of multiple literacies, yet leaves out any mention of comprehension strategies as a possible approach toward empowering adolescents (Conley, 2007). These omissions are important since both research perspectives – content area literacy and adolescent literacy – could benefit by considering the link between learning strategies and comprehension.  For content area literacy, comprehension strategies provide a purpose for instruction – to teach students, for example, how to activate prior knowledge, summarize and question, and organize information for recall and/or writing.  For adolescent literacy, comprehension strategies provide yet another form of literacy for constructing meaning within in-school and out-of-school contexts.
             The purpose of this chapter is to explore comprehension strategies as a powerful foundation for adolescent comprehension in the content areas. Previous research on comprehension strategies has been limited by its focus on younger readers and writers with only very simple tasks, such as memory and recall (Pressley& Hilden, 2006).  Much less is known about the potential for comprehension strategies that adolescents can employ to engage complex texts and tasks in the content areas. This chapter explores the potential for developing adolescents’ understanding of comprehension strategies in the content areas.
The Failure to Connect Teaching, Learning and Adolescents
            Historically, content area reading was designed to “develop students’ reading-to-learn strategies,” including locating, comprehending, remembering and retrieving information (Moore et al., 1983).  A second stated purpose was to assist students in developing “reading-to-do” strategies, which include all of the tasks that accompany content area-specific work, such as “completing labexperiments, assembling mechanical devices and following recipes.”  The original notions of content area reading placed students at the center of instruction, with the goal of helping students develop understandings of reading strategies highly correlated with achievement in the content areas (Moore et al., 1983). Moore, Readance and Rickelman’s historical review pointed to methods textbooks devoted to content area reading as evidence of these views (Moore et al., 1983).
            However, if we examine past or even current content area literacy textbooks, there is actually little, if any, evidence that content area literacy develops students’ reading-to-learn strategies or that students are necessarily at the center of instruction.  Table 1 represents a recent analysis of topics held in common among 8 popular methods textbooks in content area literacy (Alvermann & Phelps, 2002; Brozo & Simpson, 2006; McKenna & Robinson, 2001; Readance et al., 2001; Ruddell, 2007; Ryder & Graves, 1999; Unrau, 2003; Vacca & Vacca, 2004). To be sure, there are variations among the texts; some emphasize multiculturalism, English as a second language, technology or No Child Left Behind policy more or less than others and in different ways.  Table 1 represents the topics found most often among the texts.
------ Insert Table 1 about here --------
            As the table illustrates, these textbooks mostly depict instructional activities, often referred to as teaching or instructional strategies, such as graphic organizers, directed reading thinking activities, questioning (as instruction), K-W-L, Guided Reading Procedure, and text structure and other kinds of reading guides.  While some activities reference comprehension strategies that students might use, such as summarizing or questioning, the dominant representation within these textbooks is of teaching activities.  Moreover, few methods texts deliberately connect teaching activities with the development of adolescents’ comprehension strategies, particularly with different kinds of students (more versus less able readers, for instance). Rather than making connections between teaching activities, learning strategies and different students, methods texts promote general teaching activities as serving the need only to develop knowledge in a content area.  The texts do not demonstrate how teachers could use a graphic organizer or reading guide, for example, to help different students gain an understanding of how to activate prior knowledge or organize knowledge for later recall independently.
            The research reviews for content area literacy and then adolescent literacy do not improve upon this picture, preferring to treat teaching activity and the development of comprehension strategies as distinct activities. Alvermann and Moore’s (1991) review draws a distinction between “teaching strategies” which are content focused and teacher-initiated and “comprehension strategies” which are student directed and intended for building independence in reading and studying (Alvermann & Moore, 1991). Teaching strategies identified and reviewed include study guides, adjunct questions, graphic organizers, advance organizers, using text structure and comprehending main ideas.  Comprehension strategies include rehearsal (underlining, taking verbatim notes), elaborating (taking notes through paraphrasing), organizing (mapping) and comprehension monitoring (think-alouds, self-questioning).   In Moore and Alvermann’s review, teaching strategies and comprehension strategies are evaluated separately with regard to their efficacy with varying abilities of students and their comprehension. The review found that students who benefit the most from teaching strategies tend to be more able readers.  An intriguing but untested hypothesis within this research is that more able readers are able to take greater advantage of teaching strategies compared with less able readers because more able readers already understand and know how to apply comprehension strategies. To the extent that teaching is recognized as a factor in developing comprehension strategies, Moore and Alvermann do acknowledge that comprehension strategies are best taught through direct instruction, explanation and modeling.  Yet, none of the familiar content area reading teaching activities (maps, guides etc.) is implicated for their effectiveness in promoting comprehension strategies.  Again, as with Moore, Readance and Rickelman’s (1983) historical review, no connections are made between teachers’ specific use of teaching strategies and students’ development of comprehension strategies.
Bean’s (2000) review reminds the field that students are at the center of literacy instruction, focusing on developing “reading and writing skill necessary to read, comprehend and react to appropriate instructional materials in a given subject area.” (Bean, 2000). Coming 17 years after Moore, Readance andRickelman’s (1983) historical review, Bean reasserts that the students are central to the process of engaging with texts.  Bean adds yet another twist by claiming that social contexts shape comprehension, including the content areas and out of school contexts.  An implication of Bean’s critique is that all of the previous work on teaching strategies and comprehension strategies needs to be reconsidered with regard to features of different social contexts, including the complexity of beliefs and practices within different disciplines and among teachers, variations in genre and task within and across content areas and differences in students’ cultures, capabilities language, aspirations and knowledge. 
As expansive as this conceptualization is in comparison with previous research and reviews focusing on teaching activities and comprehension strategies, Bean’s contextual perspective does not provide explicit connections between teaching and learning. While students are placed definitively at the center of socially constructed meaning making, Bean does not explain how learning could or should happen.  As a result, just as much as the more cognitive-oriented views of the past do not connect teaching strategies and comprehension strategies, the social constructivist approach highlights adolescents’ social milieu without providing insight about what teachers could or should do to help them (teaching strategies) or what students could or should do to help themselves (comprehension strategies).  This ongoing omission – connecting teaching, learning and adolescents - is responsible for severely limiting what the fields of content area literacy/adolescent literacy can recommend with regard to improving adolescent comprehension.
Seeking Balance with Third Space
Moje et al.’s (2004) work with third space represents a groundbreaking attempt to return students to the center of comprehension, as envisioned by Bean, while connecting with the more cognitive point of view of strategies, promoted by Alvermann and Moore.  One could argue that Moje’s work finally delivers on Moore, Readance and Rickelman’s promise of placing adolescents at the center while teaching them how to comprehend. 
            Moje’s notion of third space involves finding ways to build bridges between everyday knowledge and discourses (ways of reading, writing and talking) and conventional academic knowledge and discourses.  In comparison with earlier accounts, Moje acknowledges a much richer view of students’ knowledge and discourse based on students’ experiences with parents’ work outside the home, work in the home, travel across countries and engagement with environmental and health issues. Her assumption is that classroom teaching and learning often ignores the students’ funds of knowledge and perspectives from home, peer groups and other networks of relationships.
            Given the history of content area literacy and adolescent literacy research, it is relatively easy to see how students’ knowledge, discourses and learning needs might be overlooked.  If texts and text-driven teaching activities are the critical variables, dominating teachers’ and their students’ attention, as Alvermann and Moore claim, students’ knowledge and literacies are often left out.  If adolescents’ multiple literacies are most important, the need for new literacy learning can be overlooked.  Moje avoids both of these pitfalls by arguing that it is not enough just to celebrate what adolescents know and can currently do.  They also need to become connected to conventional academic texts and discourses as a way of entry into disciplinary communities (such as mathematics and science) and the workplace. 
            But the story does not end here. It is not just about texts and tasks.  Mojeargues that teachers must find ways to help adolescents use their sometimes marginalized knowledge and ways of reading, writing and talking to engage themselves in conventional academic comprehension and learning.  Moje documents how a science teacher teaching about the water quality fails to build on students’ experiences with their families, including water pollution in the local community and community activism to address the problem. She also notes how students rarely volunteer what they know from home and family, because they do not see how the concepts under study are important to their lives, nor do they feel that the teacher will acknowledge what they know.
            Adopting Moje’s view means recognizing a much more complex picture of comprehension than depicted in the past research.  In fact, it is not entirely clear what comprehension instruction might look like from a perspective balanced delicately between students’ knowledge and literacies and academic texts and discourse.  Mojeoffers several principles that might characterize comprehension instruction that bridges the home and the academic.  For example, it is clear that teachers need to welcome different kinds of knowledge and discourses in the classroom.  And comprehending academic texts and engaging in discourses about them requires knowing the structure, concepts, principles and discourses of a content area.  What is less clear is how peer experiences, knowledge and discourses can be brought in alongside academic knowledge and discourses to develop students’ capacity in the content areas.  For instance, peer activities around music and popular culture equip students for critical analyses of texts.  But how can teachers rally adolescents’ critical discourses to critique classroom texts?
            The view of teaching activities and strategies o prevalent in the research and reviews within content area literacy and adolescent literacy may not be adequate for the kind of bridging Moje describes.  As Moje correctly notes, many teaching activities and comprehension strategies can be practiced in ways that are disconnected from the students or the disciplines in which they are used.  There is often the assumption that infusing generic teaching and/or learning strategies into the disciplines is the key for developing content area or adolescent literacy.  When the infusion doesn’t “take” and teachers and students complain, the teachers are labeled “resistors” (Stewart & O'Brien, 1989).  An alternative view might be that the teaching activities or comprehension strategies have not been carefully considered with regard to the disciplinary context – the structure, concepts and principles of the content area or the knowledge and discourses that students bring with them.  In a complicated disciplinary context where, as Moje suggests, students have significant knowledge and discourses to apply yet fail to speak up about it, the teachers fail to invite and recognize students’ knowledge, and the disciplines present their own unique challenges with respect to knowledge, genre and structure, the response from the field has been astonishingly simple, bordering on irrelevant.  The prevailing wisdom has been to give teachers and students graphic organizers and comprehension guides.  As Moje’s research amply demonstrates, this prevailing wisdom is nowhere nearly enough and may even confuse an already complicated set of challenges for teachers and adolescents in the content areas.
Disciplinary Views of Comprehension Strategy Instruction
            Yet another approach for adolescent comprehension is to consider disciplinary contexts –the content areas - and how learning strategies can be developed and applied appropriately.  To a large extent, the content areas, including mathematics, science, social studies and English, have been considered by content area literacy and adolescent literacy as monolithic.  That is to say, the notion that there are multiple educational traditions, sub-disciplines, multiple kinds of texts and tasks within sub-disciplines, and multiple views of students and classroom discourse has rarely if ever been acknowledged.  For instance, there is little recognition that mathematics consists of the sub-disciplines of algebra, geometry, or trigonometry or that science consists of biology, chemistry and physics.  There is no acknowledgement that disciplines like English are comprised of different educational traditions or perspectives, often in tension with one another (Applebee, 1997b).  And there is little awareness that teaching social studies or history involve different assumptions about knowledge or pedagogy (Evans, 2006).
            Treating the various disciplines as monolithic has made it easier for proponents of content area and adolescent literacy to promote generic comprehension strategies as a cure-all.  As evidence, open up virtually any methods text for teachers and the same formats and templates for different comprehension activities are replicated from one content area to the next with little regard for the particular challenges of concepts, structure, genre or task within a content area or sub-discipline.  In many teacher preparation courses, teachers are expected to make the necessary connections between content goals and teaching activities, often with very little guidance from a disciplinary point of view (Star et al., in press).
            From the disciplinary side, there are numerous perspectives on what it means to learn.  In the following sections, I review many of the predominant perspectives on learning within the disciplines of science, social studies, English and mathematics.  This review is not meant to be exhaustive nor completely representative.  Most, if not all of the perspectives on learning within the disciplines overlap as well as sometimes complement or conflict with one another.  After each disciplinary review, I explore the implications of disciplinary, sub-topic and philosophical perspectives within each of the disciplines for comprehension strategies and strategy instruction.  A key question for this review is: Given the multifaceted nature of disciplinary views of learning, what could our understanding of comprehension strategies have to offer?
Disciplinary Perspectives on Science Learning
            There are several common goals in science teaching and learning (Anderson, 2007). These include including helping students develop social agency, defined as skills and discourses that will enable them to access science related jobs, and agency in the materialworld, defined as successful interaction (observing, measuring, predicting, explaining) with the world in ways that lead to responsible stewardship and action.  Many researchers in science education also agree that most institutions of formal education do not help students learn science with understanding and that there is a persistent achievement gap in science learning that separates students by race, ethnicity and social class (Lee & Luykx, 2007).   
            Beyond these common goals and beliefs, there are at least three distinct traditions within science teaching and learning (Anderson, 2007).  Conceptual change research is the most prevalent of the research traditions. The conceptual change tradition characterizes learning problems as stemming from interactions between students’ existing knowledge and scientific concepts.  In some versions of this perspective, problems emerge from conflicts between what students have observed and come to know about the natural world and scientific discourse and understandings. 
            One recent study that illustrates conceptual change research is Scherz and Oren’s (2006) intervention to change middle school students’ images of science and technology (Scherz & Oren, 2006).  The researchers are concerned about ways in which students’ preconceptions and stereotypes of scientists and scientific work inhibit their choices about science careers.  This problem is not helped and it is even exacerbated by the fact that school science takes place in classrooms far removed from the actual work of scientists in laboratories.  The research intervention involves placing students in the role of journalists who explore a scientific subject that interests them. Students read up on background material on the subject and then go out into the field in laboratories or factories to observe and interview.  Next, students process, analyze and communicate the information they have gathered to other students.  As a result of this intervention, the researchers found that students changed their preconceptions and stereotypes of science in the workplace to reflect more informed conceptions.  Moreover, students reported greater awareness for the different types of science oriented occupations available to them, as a result of their experiences in the study.
            The sociocultural tradition is a second perspective within science education (Anderson, 2007).  Whereas conceptual change researchers focus on developing understandings of scientific knowledge and practices, sociocultural researchers are interested in the culture and language of scientific communities.  Put another way, conceptual change researchers investigate interactions with concepts about nature, while sociocultural researchers emphasize interactions among people about science.  Sociocultural researchers confront the problem of conflicts among discourses, such as students’ ways of knowing, doing, talking, reading and writing and scientific discourses.  On the one hand, based on their experiences in their family and in the community, students can enter study of a topic already familiar with some of the discourses that communities of scientists employ to interact with nature. Challenging assumptions around issues of pollution is one example of the discourse students acquire through experiences in their community (Moje et al., 2004). On the other hand, students can enter a topic with little, if any, experience with the values, social norms and ways of using language used by scientific communities.  How to acknowledge and transform students’ discourses into scientists’ language and practices, thus providing students agency in both the social and material worlds of science, is the learning problem undertaken by sociocultural science researchers.
            A recent example from the sociocultural tradition involves an investigation of authoritative and dialogic discourse for making meaning in high school science lessons (Scott et al., 2006).  Within a unit on heat, cold and temperature, the researchers explore the tension between authoritarian discourse, in which teachers focus on the school science point of view, and dialogic discourse, in which students make sense of what the teacher is saying and interact with one another to entertain different points of view. The researchers document the complex interplay of authoritarian discourse, which is important for socializing students into scientific discourse, and dialogic discourse, which is important for students to practice and internalize the tools of scientific discourse.  The researchers argue that a purposeful shifting from authoritative to dialogic discourse is necessary in order to introduce scientific discourse, problematize the content, uncover students’ knowledge and discourses, and guide students’ engagement.
            A third tradition within science teaching and learning concerns critical research (Anderson, 2007).  Critical researchers in science education assume that there is far too much emphasis placed on establishing and maintaining control over students, including their knowledge and discourse development.  They emphasize that science knowledge and discourse are privileged and the product of dominant classes. Students who are not among the privileged – the economically disadvantaged, for example – are often marginalized.  That many urban-based schools lack even the most basic materials or consistent curricula is evidence for this view (Ruby, 2006), though the exercise of power and privilege  combined with the marginalization of others appears in many other forms as well.
            An recent example of critical research in science education is Kenneth Tobin’s study about teaching chemistry to migrant 10th graders (Tobin, 2005).  Tobin notes that science, like other curricular areas, is guilty of engaging in social reproduction, producing “haves” and “have-nots” with regard to achievement.  He indicts the existing social system in which teachers and students work as well as a lack of responsiveness to culture as reasons for the achievement gap in science education. Tobin’s remedy for these problems involves teachers creating social capital and productive social networks with students.  One teacher in Tobin’s study became successful because she established a routine of meeting and informally interacting with students at the door, usually about family and social interests, guided students and encouraged them to participate and engaged in informal conversations as she monitored their progress. The teacher continually demonstrated hope for her students and worked toward their success.  Rather than focusing on a purely university-oriented view of science education, Tobin’s teacher concentrated on helping her students develop a cultural toolkit containing “science facts and concepts, the ability to read and make sense of science-related texts, and a capacity to use science to make sense of experiences in critical events in the world.” (p. 588)  Tobin concludes that only by adopting these practices – inviting participation, engaging in responsive instruction and enjoyment of learning – issues of control are replaced by a focus on science learning and expanding possibilities in science for all students and not just a few.
Comprehension Strategies in Science
            Literacy researchers have sometimes acknowledged variations in perspective about science teaching and learning, differences are noted more often among teachers than within the discipline itself (Jetton & Anderson, 1997).  The differences documented here between conceptual change, sociocultural and critical research pose the question: What are comprehension strategies that might be usefully applied from within each of these perspectives? What follows is an attempt to answer this question.
            From a conceptual change point of view, comprehension centers on interactions between existing knowledge and new scientific knowledge.  From a literacy perspective, this places a priority on at least four comprehension strategies: activating prior knowledge, predicting, questioning and summarizing. The conceptual change research concentrates on ways teachers, specialized curriculum materials and tasks can scaffold changes from naive conceptions to scientific understanding.  There has been relatively little exploration in and outside the discipline about the benefit of empowering students for conceptual change through the use of comprehension strategies..  Future research might productively explore the added value but also the added challenges of conceptual change that is not only teacher-directed but also supported by students’ use of comprehension strategies.  An important emphasis within this work must be finding ways for students to connect their use of these learning strategies with effective conceptual change as opposed to just encouraging students to lead themselves further into misconception.
            From a sociocultural view of teaching and learning science, helping students connect their existing discourses with the discourses of the scientific community is important.  Literacy researchers have only begun to understand the complexity of discourse within disciplinary contexts such as science.  Moje’s (2004) study is a good example of looking at discourse from a literacy perspective where it becomes clear that students’ discourses from home and community are both essential and often ignored (Moje et al., 2004). The implication here is that comprehension strategies in science need to be considered with regard to discipline-specific conceptual goals, the discourse resources of students and the desired scientific discourses, in order to be successful.  While it has been popular to recommend generic discussion activities for comprehension in content areas for a very long time (Alvermann, 1987), the sociocultural perspective in science illustrates that generic approaches run the risk of ignoring conceptual goals while failing to capitalize on and transform students’ discourse into scientific discourse and understanding.
            From a critical research point of view within science, acknowledging students’ cultural capital, encouraging participation, and responsiveness to students are all important.  And so, comprehension needs to be considered with regard to building rapport with students, inviting what they know and have experienced, and guiding them toward a greater potential for understanding and interacting with the material world.  This perspective is grounded more in dispositions needed for effective comprehension than in concept or pedagogy.  While literacy researchers have engaged themselves in critical research around comprehension, most often this research is concerned with identity formation or generic discussion practices, sometimes to the exclusion of disciplinary learning (Sutherland, 2005).  As the critical research in science education traditions suggests, however, acknowledging students’ identity and providing them with tools for understanding science can be the keys to empowering students in the classroom and beyond. As Pressley has noted, building motivation and positive dispositions toward learning are the best ways for creating conditions for learning strategy instruction (Pressley, 2006).  The critical research view in science could provide a rich context for literacy researchers to connect adolescent identity and critical discourse with comprehension in the particular context of science teaching and learning.
Disciplinary Perspectives on Learning in Social Studies
            Disciplinary perspectives in social studies and history are as diverse as they are in science and have equally diverse implications for comprehension instruction.  Evans (2006) documents the multiple traditions within social studies education as well as the swings in emphasis over the past century.  There are the traditional historians who see the purpose of social studies as the acquisition of content knowledge about history, including mastery of chronologies and textbook-based learning (Leming et al., 2003).  There are the social scientists who see social studies as teaching the social science disciplines, including sociology, economics, education, geography and the law.   Social efficiency educators focus on the world of tomorrow with an emphasis on business and industry.  There are the social meliorists who seek to develop students’ thinking about how to improve society.  And finally, there are the social reconstructivists who teach students to critique the status quo and create a more just society. 
            More recently, there have been calls to bring all of these perspectives together to create more shared understandings (Wineburg et al., 2007). However, these efforts have opened up even more complexity among these multiple perspectives with regard to teaching and learning.  Rather than reducing this complexity and thereby selling short the potential of teaching and learning social studies, social studies educators are attempting to build productively on the tensions among the perspectives.  So, for example, social studies educators embrace the insights from more student-centered perspectives that adolescents’ experiences, ideas and understandings matter when it comes to historical thinking, while, at the same time, considering the thinking processes of working historians as well.
            VanSledright (2004) compares the historical thinking of novices with the thinking of historians (VanSledright, 2004). The result is the identification of learning strategies unique to historical thinking.  VanSledright proposes that one of the jobs of social studies education should be to close the gap between adolescent novices and expert historians. Doing this involves teaching novices the learning strategies practiced by historians.  VanSledright focuses his efforts on the source work of historians as a form of critical literacy.  Sources – documents, maps historical accounts – all represent remnants of the past selected and organized from someone’s perspective.  Historians create their understanding of history based on sources and their own questions.  Four strategies useful in learning from sources include: identification, or figuring out what a source is in the context of type, appearance and timeframe; attribution, or recognizing that a source is constructed by a particular author for particular purposes in particular times and contexts; judging perspective, or judging an author’s social, cultural and political position; and judging reliability, or comparing one account with other accounts from a historical period.
Comprehension Strategies in Social Studies
            In the rare cases where comprehension in considered in the context of social studies, the approach is almost universally from the outside-in.  Put another way, literacy researchers create their own assumptions about the kinds of comprehension that should be taught and learned and then examine social studies classrooms, teacher and student interactions and textbooks to determine whether desirable comprehension practices are occurring.  In some cases, this approach leads to conclusions that little or no comprehension instruction is happening (Armbruster & Gudbrandsen, 1986).  In other cases, a model for comprehension instruction is posited accompanied by claims that the model can be used “across the social studies genres – textbooks, primary sources, fictional texts or a combination (Massey & Heafner, 2004). In either case, the multifaceted nature of social studies as a discipline and as a context for learning is overlooked.
            And so, what are comprehension strategies from a social studies perspective?  Answering this question requires acknowledging the multiple perspectives within social studies.  If social studies is treated as a cognitive act of acquiring more knowledge, then the most important learning strategies might involve forming connections among various kinds of knowledge.  If social studies is multidisciplinary, then there are probably different learning strategies appropriate to learning in sociology, economics, education, geography and the law.  As the social studies research suggests, each sub-discipline reflects different kinds of challenges and opportunities for learning.  If social studies is about building worlds of tomorrow, then prediction might be most important for comprehension.  If social studies is about improving or uprooting society, then comprehension strategies that represent a more critical edge, such as questioning, might be more effective.
            The more compelling view, however, may be that, as social studies educators themselves have discovered, all of these views are collectively important. This translates into the challenging notion for literacy researchers that social studies represents a considerably more complex world for comprehension, in which comprehension strategies are not generalizable across all texts, tasks and contexts.  Selling generalized strategies in social studies poses two problems, (1) forcing social studies teachers to make their own specific instructional adaptations appropriate to already complicated contexts and (2) raising the possibility that retrofitting comprehension strategies to the discipline might neglect or gloss over some very important disciplinary dimensions. Contrary to the long-held assumption that teachers’ abandonment of comprehension strategies implies their resistance (Stewart & O'Brien, 1989), for social studies teachers, it could just mean that they are confronting the complexity of the discipline while realizing that over-generalized approaches to comprehension in the social studies just won’t work.
            A possible way to proceed to connect literacy and social studies would be for literacy researchers to carefully examine the implications of multiple views of social studies and multiple views of historical thinking as a starting point for considering comprehension strategies.  This means abandoning the assumption that comprehension is the same across all texts and contexts but also wading into the messy world of social studies teaching and learning.
Disciplinary Perspectives on Learning in English
            There has been an ongoing tension in English education between those who want English to be about acquiring knowledge versus those who consider English education as acquiring ways of knowing.  Put simply, the tension is between advocates of content and advocates of process (Applebee, 1997a).  There is a third position which attempts to balance concerns for content and concerns for process by considering significant content in the context of engaging activity both from a teaching and a learning perspective (Applebee, 1996).
            Some of the most ardent proponents of content acquisition come from outside English education, such as E.D. Hirsh.  Hirsch contends that there is a definite body of literacy knowledge that underlies what it means to be educated in American culture and society (Hirsch, 2006).  There are also proponents of content acquisition from inside the profession when it comes to cognitive views of writing instruction, with research-based claims about what adolescents need to know and know how to do to write well (Graham & Perin, 2007; Hillocks, 2005). At the other end of spectrum are those who question the cannon –  privileged knowledge about literature and writing – in favor of, respectively, world literature (Power, 2003; Reese, 2002), literature reflecting diversity in human condition and experience (Blackburn, 2003; Goebel, 2004; Ressler, 2005), or literature produced for and by adolescents (Morrell, 2004; Schwarz, 2006). 
            The process perspective on English education offers up equally diverse points of view.  There are those who advocate for English as a pathway toward more effective communication (Berger, 2005); those who see English as a way to promote active citizenship, democracy and social justice (Mantle-Bromley & Foster, 2005); those who view English as a transaction between native language and culture and second language and culture (Cruz, 2004; Gutierrez & Orellana, 2006); and there are those who see English as the nexus for engaging in new literacies (Street, 2005).  Attempts to balance content and process are evidenced by approaches which connect, for example, various kinds of literature with process goals (Whiten, 2005).
            English education, like the other disciplines, also represents sociocultural and discourse perspectives.  From the sociocultural side, there are studies of gender identity (Fairbanks & Ariail, 2006) and racial and cultural identity combined with concerns about marginalization in school and the need to acknowledge the intellectual and social capital that adolescents bring to the learning of English (Trainor, 2005).  Starting out with concerns about literary interpretation, the discourse perspective explores tensions and conflicts among classroom participants as they struggle to shape understandings of literature and writing (Nystrand, 2006; Smagorinsky et al., 1994).  Bridging the cognitive and the sociocultural, the discourse research has reported positive gains in achievement in discussion based classrooms with desirable features, including open exchange among students, authentic, open-ended questions, and follow-up questions (Applebee et al., 2003).
Comprehension Strategies in English
            The diversity of theory, research and praxis within English education defies the application of any one or even a set of comprehension strategies.  However, the content acquisition perspective might benefit from a focus on any one of a number of comprehension strategies, such as those identified by Deshler and his colleagues for students with learning disabilities (Bulgren, 2006; Bulgren & Scanlon, 1998; Deshler et al., 2001).  Content enhancement and concept comparison strategies have been validated with students with learning disabilities and among students in inclusive classrooms in a range of content area disciplines. There are also proven strategies for improving adolescents’ knowledge and performance with writing (Graham, 2006; Troia & Graham, 2002).
            Comprehension strategies to enhance the various process perspectives within English are more difficult to identify, in part, because there can be little agreement about what it means for adolescents to learn how to “do” English differently or more effectively.  Comprehension strategies that engage adolescents in asking and answering their own questions could prove useful when the mission is to encourage tools for democratic action and social justice.  Activating prior knowledge might be important for helping adolescents connect their language and culture to understandings of new languages and cultures.  The generic ways in which these comprehension strategies have been researched and promoted limits their utility without substantial extrapolation and adaptation to specific contexts and needs.
            Identity, gender, social capital, power and marginalization issues raised from a sociocultural perspective are not so easily addressed with a focus on comprehension strategies.  The issues here are about who is asking the questions and for what purposes.  Comprehension strategies within the sociocultural mix become very quickly enmeshed in concerns for who is teaching the students how to comprehend and for what agenda(s)? The ultimate, desirable goal, from this point of view might be that all adolescents learn comprehension strategies in ways that help them in their quest for identity, aiding them in resisting marginalization, while promoting their assets among peers and adults
            The discourse perspective is already accompanied by a set of strategies, including question asking, following up on responses and engaging in conversation. Research on comprehension strategies adds a concern that adolescents require specific kinds of explanation, modeling and feedback to productively engage in these strategies for discourse and comprehension (Pressley & Hilden, 2006).
Disciplinary Perspectives on Learning in Mathematics
            Mathematics bears some resemblance to science in that there are cognitive perspectives, sociocultural perspectives and critical discourse perspectives.  All three perspectives in mathematics share a concern for students’ ideas, experiences and interests, yet all three perspectives portray a different view of the nature and role of what student bring to mathematics learning.
            From a cognitive point of view, the concern is for the types of mathematical knowledge children understand before and throughout their years of schooling (Siegler, 2003).  Many children come to school already knowledgeable about numbers and mathematical concepts and principles.  But many other children fail to understand concepts and principles that are basic to understanding more abstract ideas, and many confront problems when they are unable to make important connections among mathematical concepts.  Even more problematic are ways in which children can generate flawed conceptions or mathematical procedures that can be very difficult to correct or unlearn. For instance, in a study of proportional reasoning, adolescents are provided with basic details of the amount of paint required to paint an irregular figure, a representation of Santa Claus.  Next, they were asked to estimate the amount of paint required to paint the irregular figure, only now it is three times the size of an original figure.  Most adolescents guessed the proportional answer, that it required three times the amount of paint, when, in reality, the answer is non-proportional, requiring just twice the amount of paint. In addition, students who engaged in the inappropriate reasoning indicated substantial certainty about the correctness of their answers.
            The problems tackled within a sociocultural perspective on mathematics include creating classroom contexts for students to develop multiple mathematical literacies and connect mathematics to their developing identity (Cobb, 2004).  This perspective is fueled in part by the realization that while many adolescents can be succeeding in mathematics, they choose not to continue their study because of conflicts between who they want to become and the expectations within the mathematics classroom.  In one version of this work, adolescents were asked to make judgments about mathematical problems taken from in-school and out-of-school contexts (Jurdak, 2006).  Adolescents addressed problems situated in school contexts by using in school mathematical tools, rules and norms while adolescents applied social and personal rules to problems situated in out-of-school contexts.  This raises a dilemma about how to relate adolescents’ problem solving models that are developed and applied in school to real world mathematical problems.  Given the modern curriculum demands for more cognitively oriented mathematical achievement, the solution to this dilemma may not be as easy as inserting more real world mathematical problems into the curriculum. On the other hand, ignoring real world problems runs the risk of  promoting a disconnect between in-school mathematics and adolescent identity and aspirations.
            A third perspective within mathematics education concerns discourse.  Like the concerns in science and literacy over discourse, this perspective focuses on engaging adolescents’ existing discourses with mathematical discourses (Sfard, 2001).  Sfard and others argue that discourse is the key to adolescent thinking, particularly with regard to building and using models of mathematical concepts and principles. Like the socioculturalists in mathematics, the discourse perspective advances the notion that the separation between school mathematics and real world mathematics is problematic. Where they differ is in the focus on context, as with the sociocultural view, versus communicative discourse, from the discourse view.  And so the problem from the discourse perspective in mathematics concerns how to develop mathematical understandings and discourse through language.  Sfard carefully documents adolescents’ use of language to develop increasingly complex ways of thinking about mathematics, first with respect to labels and then onto abstract representations.  It is often more comfortable for adolescents to use their everyday language to resolve conflicts. However, their use of everyday language can also become a pathway toward incomplete or flawed understandings.
Comprehension Strategies in Mathematics
            In a recent review of content area literacy from a mathematics perspective, Star and his colleagues warn about the danger of literacy instruction in mathematics being devoid of mathematics learning (Star et al., in press).  Star notes that many of the content area literacy textbooks prescribe activities for reading the mathematics textbook, something that “reflects a very limited understanding of mathematics texts as a unique genre.”  Mathematics texts are often referred to as containing worked examples with sentences sprinkled in. Mathematics educators all have something different to say about the problems posed by mathematics texts from a cognitive, sociocultural or discourse perspective.  However, it has never been clear how the teaching activities within content area or adolescent literacy would be in any way helpful with the problems in mathematical learning identified from within each of these perspectives.  This leads us back to the question: What are comprehension strategies that might potentially be productive in mathematics?
              Some mathematics education researchers go so far as to argue that comprehension is not a problem in mathematics learning (Mayer, 2004). Word problems, for example, require specific learning strategies, including translating, or converting individual sentences into internal mental representations; integrating, or building a model (selecting important information, making interpretations) of the problem situation represented by the problem; solution planning or monitoring, or devising a step by step plan for solving a word problem; and solution execution, or carrying out a plan for solving the problem.  Many students are able to translate and integrate word problems, in effect, comprehending the problems.  Yet many students are unable to plan or carry out solutions to the problems either because they have little experience with the mathematics concepts or the word problems, or they haven’t yet learned productive strategies for planning and executing solutions.
            From a literacy point of view, two comprehension strategies would appear to be helpful with these mathematics strategies, summarizing and predicting.  It might be productive for adolescents to ask periodically What do I know now?, or What will happen if I try this solution?  The history of content area literacy or adolescent literacy has not considered ways to relate comprehension strategies to the unique demands of mathematical problem solving. So we know very little about what would happen by marrying concerns for mathematical learning with what we know about comprehension. On the other hand, the potential exists for finding a way to promote content area learning in mathematics and literacy together, rather than trading off one goal (disciplinary knowledge) for another (literacy).
            The same kinds of concerns surround literacy and sociocultural and discourse perspectives in mathematics.  While the content literacy/adolescent literacy perspectives promote the importance of context, multiple literacies and discourse, the research has neglected the particular challenges of mathematics contexts and discourses.  The literacy research recognizes that activating prior knowledge is generally important, but says little, if anything, about the role of prior knowledge in mathematical model building, making connections between school and out of school mathematics, or using discourse to build mathematical understandings and confront misconceptions.
Implications for Research and Practice
            As this review demonstrates, content area literacy and adolescent literacy started out with good intentions - to place adolescents at the center of instruction and to build their capacity to study and learn in the content areas.  However, as this review also demonstrates, the original mission very quickly went awry by generating compendia after cornucopia of general teaching activities, most of which are connected to gaining knowledge and only a very few of which have anything to do with comprehension strategy learning.  The shift to adolescent literacy engaged the literacy profession in exploration and celebration of adolescence, but did little to address the problem of the ongoing disconnect between adolescents, literacy learning and the disciplines.  Moje’s research is the notable departure from tradition, at once bringing together insights about adolescents, multiple literacies and discourses in a content area context (Moje et al., 2004).  Still, her work leaves us with many questions for how the literacy profession can deliberately proceed to strengthen our understanding of these connections.
            Important clues for how adolescents might develop comprehension strategies productive in the content areas can be found in the diverse perspectives coming from within the content area disciplines.  The history within content area literacy and adolescent literacy of offering up generic teaching activities for monolithic views of the disciplines has hindered our view of what might be possible.  As this chapter illustrates, there are a number of potential connections but only if literacy researchers recognize the multifaceted nature of the content areas, including the subtopics and perspectives on learning.
            When it comes to content area literacy and adolescent literacy, the research traditions are very familiar – do the comprehension research and then apply it to content area practice.  This prevailing paradigm supports the flawed practice of researching comprehension and then infusing the findings into the content areas without considering what makes learning in content area contexts both diverse and often challenging.  To understand comprehension strategies and how they might work in the content areas, researchers need to reverse this pattern and do what Herber called for many years ago - study disciplinary practice and research comprehension within disciplinary contexts (Herber & Nelson-Herber, 1981). Literacy researchers need to go from practice into research, rather than fitting isolated and decontexualized comprehension research into disciplinary practice, if we are ever to understand comprehension strategies in the disciplines.
            For practice, this discussion raises a whole set of new possibilities for considering the contribution of comprehension strategies for learning in the content areas.  The content areas pose many learning problems that literacy researchers have never envisioned or explored. Nuances of scientific understanding, purposes for and pitfalls in learning various science and mathematics concepts, issues of bias and point of view in source materials in social studies, and the multiple and sometimes conflicting goals of literature learning and writing instruction in English present a complex but fertile landscape for better understanding comprehension strategy instruction and learning. 
            In some cases, the existing milieu of teaching activities within content area literacy and adolescent literacy might be usefully applied in these contexts. In some cases, research and practice will need to be more inventive, developing new forms of comprehension teaching and learning.  We need to consider the potential of any practice for teaching comprehension strategies at all.  The all-too-typical pattern of rehearsing adolescents through questioning, summarization and predicting activities offers the illusion of comprehension instruction but does not build an understanding of comprehension strategies. Teaching comprehension strategies requires explanation, modeling, feedback and practice (Pressley, 2006).  The future for improving adolescent comprehension requires a much better understanding for how teachers can help adolescents and adolescent can help themselves understand and apply comprehension strategies to learn successfully in the content areas.
















References
Alvermann, D. (1987). Using discussion to promote reading comprehension. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
Alvermann, D., & Moore, D. (1991). Secondary school reading. In R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research, volume ii (pp. 951-983). New York: Longman.
Alvermann, D., & Phelps, S. (2002). Content reading and literacy: Succeeding in today's diverse classrooms (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Anderson, C. (2007). Perspectives on science learning. In S. Abell & N. Lederman (Eds.), Handbook of research on science education (pp. 3-30). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Applebee, A. (1996). Curriculum as conversation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Applebee, A. (1997). Rethinking curriculum in the english language arts. English Journal, 86(5), 25-31.
Applebee, A., Langer, J., Nystrand, M., & Gamoran, A. (2003). Discussion-based approaches to developing understanding: Classroom instruction and student performance in middle and high school. American Educational Research Journal, 40, 685-730.
Armbruster, B., & Gudbrandsen, B. (1986). Reading comprehension instruction in the social studies. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(1), 36-48.
Bean, T. (2000). Reading in the content areas: Social constructivist dimensions. In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research, volume iii (pp. 629-644). New York: Longman.
Berger, J. (2005). Transforming writers through grammar study. English Journal, 95(5), 53-60.
Blackburn, M. (2003). Exploring literacy performances and power dynamics at the loft: Queer youth reading the world and the word. Research in the Teaching of English, 37(4), 467-490.
Block, C., Gambrell, L., & Pressley, M. (2002). Improving comprehension instruction: Rethinking research, theory and classroom practice. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Brozo, W., & Simpson, M. (2006). Content literacy for today's adolescents. New York: Prentice Hall.
Bulgren, J. (2006). Integrated content enhancement routines: Responding to the needs of adolescents with disabilities in rigorous inclusive secondary content classes. Teaching Exceptional Children, 38(6), 54-58.
Bulgren, J., & Scanlon, D. (1998). Instructional routines and learning strategies that promote understanding of content area concepts. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 41(4), 292-302.
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J. (2004). Reading comprehension difficulties. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Handbook of children's literacy (pp. 313-338). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.
Cobb, P. (2004). Mathematics, literacies and identity. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(3), 333-337.
Conley, M. (2007). Reconsidering adolescent literacy: From competing agendas to shared commitment. In M. Pressley (Ed.), Research we have, research we need. New York: Guilford.
Cruz, M. (2004). Can english language learners acquire academic english? English Journal, 93(4), 14-19.
Deshler, D., Schumaker, B., Lenz, K., Bulgren, J., Hock, M., Knight, J., et al. (2001). Ensuring content-area learning by secondary students with learning disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16(2), 96-108.
Evans, R. (2006). The social studies wars: Now and then. Social Education, 70(5), 317-322.
Fairbanks, C., & Ariail, M. (2006). The role of social and cultural resources in literacy: Three contrasting cases. Research in the Teaching of English, 40(3), 310-354.
Goebel, B. (2004). An ethical approach to teaching native american literature. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Graham, S. (2006). Strategy instruction and the teaching of writing. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 187-207). New York: Guilford.
Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007). Writing next: Effective strategies to improve writing of adolescents in middle and high schools. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.
Gutierrez, K., & Orellana, M. (2006). The 'problem' of english learners: Constructing genres of difference. Research in the Teaching of English, 40(4), 502-507.
Herber, H., & Nelson-Herber, J. (1981). Practice into research: The other half of the formula. Paper presented at the Second Annual Conference on Reading Research, New Orleans.
Hillocks, G. (2005). The focus of form vs. Content in teaching writing. Research in the Teaching of English, 40(2), 238-248.
Hirsch, E. D. (2006). The knowledge deficit. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
Jetton, T., & Anderson, P. (1997). Instructional importance: What teachers value and what students learn. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(3), 290-308.
Jurdak, M. (2006). Contrasting perspectives and performance of high school students on problem solving in real world, situated and school contexts. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 63(3), 238-301.
Lee, O., & Luykx, A. (2007). Science education and student diversity: Race/ethnicity, language, culture, and socioeconomic status. In S. Abell & N. Lederman (Eds.), Handbook of research on science education. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Leming, J., Ellington, L., & Porter, K. (2003). Where did social studies go wrong? Washington, DC: Fordham Foundation.
Mantle-Bromley, C., & Foster, A. (2005). Educating for democracy: The vital role of the language arts teacher. English Journal, 94(5), 70-75.
Massey, D., & Heafner, T. (2004). Promoting reading comprehension in social studies. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 48(1), 26-41.
Mayer, R. (2004). Teaching of subject matter. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 715-744.
McKenna, M., & Robinson, R. (1990). Content literacy: A definition and implications. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 34(3), 184-186.
McKenna, M., & Robinson, R. (2001). Teaching through text: Reading and writing in the content areas. New York: Allyn and Bacon.
Moje, E., Ciechanowski, K., Kramer, K., Ellis, L., Carrillo, R., & Collazo, T. (2004). Working toward third space in content area literacy: An examination of everyday funds of knowledge. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(1), 38-70.
Moje, E., Dillon, D., & O'Brien, D. (2000). Reexamining roles of learner, text, and context in secondary literacy. The Journal of Educational Research, 93(3), 165-180.
Moore, D., Readance, J., & Rickelman, R. (1983). An historical exploration of content area reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 18(4), 419-438.
Morrell, E. (2004). Linking literacy and popular culture. Norwood, MA: Christopher Gordon.
Nystrand, M. (2006). Classroom discourse and reading comprehension. Research in the Teaching of English, 40(4), 392-412.
Power, C. (2003). Challenging the pluralism of our past: Presentism and the selective tradition in historical fiction written for young people. Research in the Teaching of English, 37(4), 425-466.
Pressley, M. (2000). What should comprehension instruction be the instruction of? In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, P. Pearson & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. III, pp. 545-561.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pressley, M. (2006). Reading instruction that works: The case for balanced teaching. New York: Guildford.
Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocals of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Pressley, M., & Hilden, K. (2006). Cognitive strategies: Production deficiencies and successful strategy instruction everywhere. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 2). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley and Sons.
Readance, J., Bean, T., & Baldwin, S. (2001). Content area literacy: An integrated approach. Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt Publishing.
Reese, J. (2002). Learning for understanding: The role of world literature. English Journal, 91(5), 63-69.
Ressler, P. (2005). Challenging normative sexual and gender identity beliefs through romeo and juliet. English Journal, 95(1), 52-58.
Ruby, A. (2006). Improving science achievement at high-poverty urban middle schools. Science, 90(6), 1005-1027.
Ruddell, M. (2007). Teaching content area reading and writing. New York: Wiley.
Ryder, R., & Graves, M. (1999). Reading and learning in content areas. New York: John Wiley and Sons.
Scherz, Z., & Oren, M. (2006). How to change students' images of science and technology. Science, 90(6), 965-985.
Schwarz, G. (2006). Expanding literacies through graphic novels. English Journal, 95(6), 58-65.
Scott, P., Mortimer, E., & Aguiar, O. (2006). The tension between authoritative and dialogic discourse: A fundamental characteristic of meaning making interactions in high school science lessons. Science, 90(4), 605-631.
Sfard, A. (2001). Learning mathematics as developing a discourse. In R. Speiser & W. Maher (Eds.), Proceedings of the 21st conference of pme-na. Columbus, Ohio: Clearinghouse for science, mathematics and environmental education.
Siegler, R. (2003). Implications of cognitive science research for mathematics education. In J. Kilpatrick, W. Martin & D. Schifter (Eds.), A research companion to principles and standards for school mathematics (pp. 219-233). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Smagorinsky, P., Smith, M., & Marshall, J. (1994). The language of interpretation: Patterns of discourse in discussions of literature. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.
Star, J., Strickland, S., & Hawkins, A. (in press). What is mathematical literacy?   Exploring the relationship between literacy and content learning in middle and high school mathematics. In M. Conley, J. Freidhoff, M. Sherry & S. Tuckey (Eds.), Adolescent literacy policy and instruction: The research we have and the research we need. New York: Guilford.
Stevens, L. (2002). Making the road by walking: The transition from content area literacy to adolescent literacy. Reading Research and Instruction, 41(3), 267-278.
Stewart, R., & O'Brien, D. (1989). Resistance to content area reading:  A focus on preservice teachers. Journal of Reading, 32(5), 396-401.
Street, B. (2005). Recent applications to new literacy studies in educational contexts. Research in the Teaching of English, 39(4), 417-423.
Sutherland, L. (2005). Black adolescent girls' use of literacy practices to negotiate boundaries of ascribed identity. Journal of Literacy Research, 37(3), 365-406.
Thorndike, E. (1917). Reading as reasoning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8(6), 323-330.
Tierney, R., & Readance, J. (2004). Reading strategies and practices: A compendium. New York: Allyn and Bacon.
Tobin, K. (2005). Building enacted science on the capital of learners. Science, 89(4), 577-594.
Trainor, J. (2005). 'my ancestors didn't own slaves': Understanding white talk about race. Research in the Teaching of English, 40(2), 140-167.
Troia, G., & Graham, S. (2002). The effectiveness of a highly explicit, teacher-directed strategy instruction routine: Changing the writing performance of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35(4), 290-305.
Unrau, N. (2003). Content area reading and writing: Fostering literacies in middle and high school cultures. New York: Prentice Hall.
Vacca, R., & Vacca, J. (2004). Content area reading: Literacy and learning across the curriculum. New York: Allyn and Bacon.
VanSledright, B. (2004). What does it mean to think historically and how do you teach it? Social Education, 68, 220-233.
Whiten, P. (2005). The interplay of text, talk and visual representation in expanding literary conversation. Research in the Teaching of English, 39(4), 365-397.
Wineburg, S., Stearns, P., & Seixas, P. (2007). Knowing, teaching and learning history. New York: New York University Press.
Wyatt, D., Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P., Stein, S., Evans, P., & Brown, R. (1993). Comprehension strategies, worth and credibility monitoring and evaluations: Cold and hot cognition when experts read professional articles that are important to them. Learning and Individual Differences, 5, 49-72.

0 Response to "CONTOH SKRIPSI BAHASA INGGRIS IMPROVING ADOLESCENT COMPREHENSION: DEVELOPING COMPREHENSION STRATEGIES IN THE CONTENT AREAS"

Posting Komentar

wdcfawqafwef

BACKLINK OTOMATIS GRATIS JURAGAN.